The Specifics of Oculomotor Activity in Children With Hearing Impairment in the Independent and Joint Performance Process of a Training Task With an Adult
PDF
PDF (Russian)

Keywords

joint attention
divided attention
education
lifespan development
preschool age
anormogenesis
hearing impairment
cochlear implantation
oculography
eye tracker

Abstract

Abstract: Introduction. The transitional status of a sample of children with hearing impairment after cochlear implantation expands the possibilities of studying the consequences of violations of individual sensory systems affecting the joint attention deficit. The method of registering eye movement has advantages in studying the difficulties of learning processes in children with hearing impairment. The novelty of the study lies in the synchronous tracking of eye movements with superimposed gaze paths of two experimental participants (an adult and a child with hearing impairment) in a learning situation and the use of a time marker of simultaneous fixations as an indicator of joint attention. Based on the data of oculomotor activity during the independent and synchronous performance of an educational task with an adult, the specifics of visual attention that hinder the learning of children with hearing impairment can be identified. Methods. Study sample: 16 preschoolers with hearing impairment (sensorineural hearing loss, class H90 according to ICD-11) and 16 typically developing children. Experimental situations of independent and synchronous performance of an educational task with an adult are involved. The leading method was to register eye movement with a portable PLabs eye tracker. Results. When performing a task synchronously with an adult in oculomotor activity in children with hearing impairment, the number of fixations indicating constant joint attention increases (from 300 milliseconds), there is greater relevance of fixations and an increase in the duration of maintaining visual attention to educational samples, as a result, a decrease in errors. Compared to typically developing children, the speed of information processing and the allocation of targeted stimuli changes in children with hearing impairment. Discussion. In episodes of joint attention in a learning situation, the synchronicity of interaction between a child with hearing impairment and an adult is achieved through fixations lasting from 300–500 and above 500 milliseconds (ms). However, the ability to maintain this joint attention in children with hearing impairment is less than in typically developing peers.

https://doi.org/10.21702/rpj.2022.3.5
PDF
PDF (Russian)

References

Abrahamson, D., & Sánchez-García, R. (2016). Learning is moving in new ways: The ecological dynamics of mathematics education. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 25(2), 203–239. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508406.2016.1143370

Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Suma, K., & Robins, D. L. (2019). An expanded view of joint attention: Skill, engagement, and language in typical development and autism. Child Development, 90(1), e1–e18. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12973

Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55(4), 1278–1289. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129997

Baron-Cohen, S., & Cross, P. (1992). Reading the eyes: Evidence for the role of perception in the development of a theory of mind. Mind & Language, 7(1–2), 172–186. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00203.x

Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–543. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00445.x

Campbell, R., Elgar, K., Kuntsi, J., Akers, R., Terstegge, J., Coleman, M., & Skuse, D. (2002). The classification of ‘fear’ from faces is associated with face recognition skill in women. Neuropsychologia, 40(6), 575–584. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00164-6

Chen, C.-h., Castellanos, I., & Yu, C., & Houston, D. M. (2020). What leads to coordinated attention in parent-toddler interactions? Children’s hearing status matters. Development Science, 23(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12919

Chen, C.-h., Castellanos, I., Yu, C., & Houston, D. M. (2019). Effects of children’s hearing loss on the synchrony between parents’ object naming and children’s attention. Infant Behavior and Development, 57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.04.004

Chen, C.-h., Houston, D. M., & Yu, C. (2021). Parent–child joint behaviors in novel object play create high-quality data for word learning. Child Development, 92(5), 1889–1905. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13620

Dalmaso, M., Galfano, G., Tarqui, L., Forti, B., & Castelli, L. (2013). Is social attention impaired in schizophrenia? Gaze, but not pointing gestures, is associated with spatial attention deficits. Neuropsychology, 27(5), 608–613. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033518

Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271

de Villiers, P. A. (2005). The role of language in theory-of-mind development: What deaf children tell us. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 266–297). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0013

Delgado, C. E. F., Peter, M., Crowson, M., Markus, J., Yale, M., & Schwartz, H. (2002). Responding to joint attention and language development: A comparison of target locations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(4), 715–719. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/057)

Duijzer, C. A. C. G., Shayan, S., Bakker, A., Van der Schaaf, M. F., & Abrahamson, D. (2017). Touchscreen tablets: Coordinating action and perception for mathematical cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00144

Dunn, J., & Brophy, M. (2005). Communication, relationships, and individual differences in children’s understanding of mind. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 50–69). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0003

Fagan, M. K., Bergeson, T. R., & Morris, K. J. (2014). Synchrony, complexity and directiveness in mothers’ interactions with infants pre- and post-cochlear implantation. Infant Behavior and Development, 37(3), 249–257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.04.001

Franco, F., & Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23(2), 307–336. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008813

Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2005). New developments in social interdependence theory. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 131(4), 285–358. https://doi.org/10.3200/MONO.131.4.285-358

Langdon, R., Corner, T., McLaren, J., Ward, P. B., & Coltheart, M. (2006). Externalizing and personalizing biases in persecutory delusions: The relationship with poor insight and theory-of-mind. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(5), 699–713. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.03.012

Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74(4), 1130–1144. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00597

Marotta, A., Román-Caballero, R., & Lupiáñez, J. (2018). Arrows don’t look at you: Qualitatively different attentional mechanisms triggered by gaze and arrows. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(6), 2254–2259. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1457-2

Meadow-Orlans, K. P. (1997). Effects of mother and infant hearing status on interactions at twelve and eighteen months. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(1), 26–36. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.deafed.a014307

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind–mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73(6), 1715–1726. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00501

Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x

Morales, M., Mundy, P., Crowson, M. M., Neal, A. R., & Delgado, C. E. F. (2005). Individual differences in infant attention skills, joint attention, and emotion regulation behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(3), 259–263. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650250444000432

Mundy, P. (2018). A review of joint attention and social-cognitive brain systems in typical development and autism spectrum disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47(6), 497–514.

Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x

Musselman, C., & Churchill, A. (1992). The effects of maternal conversational control on the language and social development of deaf children. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 14(2), 99–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/152574019201400201

Nichols, K. E., Fox, N., & Mundy, P. (2005). Joint attention, self-recognition, and neurocognitive function in toddlers. Infancy, 7(1), 35–51. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0701_4

Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V. (2003). Opening windows into the mind: Mothers’ preferences for mental state explanations and children’s theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 18(3), 399–429. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00041-8

Redcay, E., Kleiner, M., & Saxe, R. (2012). Look at this: The neural correlates of initiating and responding to bids for joint attention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00169

Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2007). The art of conversation is coordination. Psychological Science, 18(5), 407–413. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01914.x

Ristic, J., Mottron, L., Friesen, C. K., Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., & Kingstone, A. (2005). Eyes are special but not for everyone: The case of autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(3), 715–718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.007

Ruff, H. A., & Lawson, K. R. (1990). Development of sustained, focused attention in young children during free play. Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 85–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.1.85

Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Rowlandson, K., Rumsey, C., & Garnham, A. (2003). How language relates to belief, desire, and emotion understanding. Cognitive Development, 18(2), 139–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00002-9

Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78(2), 376–396. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x

Schroer, S. E., & Yu, C. (2021). The sensorimotor dynamics of joint attention. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 43. Escholarship. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/2kn7k904

Shvarts, A. (2018). Joint attention in resolving the ambiguity of different presentations: A dual eye-tracking study of the teaching learning process. In N. Presmeg, L. Radford, W.-M. Roth, G. Kadunz (Eds.), Signs of signification: Semiotics in mathematics education research (pp. 73–103). Springer.

Smith, L., & Ulvund, S. E. (2003). The role of joint attention in later development among preterm children: Linkages between early and middle childhood. Social Development, 12(2), 222–234. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00230

Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P. C., Acra, C. F., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E. F., Parlade, M. V., Meyer, J. A., Neal, A. R., & Pomares, Y. B. (2007). Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. Child Development, 78(1), 53–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00985.x

Vlamings, P. H. J. M., Stauder, J. E. A., van Son, I. A. M., & Mottron, L. (2005). Atypical visual orienting to gaze- and arrow-cues in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 267–277. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-3289-y

Woolfe, T., Want, S. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Signposts to development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child Development, 73(3), 768–778. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00437

Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2017a). Hand-eye coordination predicts joint attention. Child Development, 88(6), 2060–2078.

Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2017b). Multiple sensory-motor pathways lead to coordinated visual attention. Cognitive Science, 41(S1). https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12366

Creative Commons License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Copyright (c) 2022 Smirnova Ya. K.