Effects of the Moderated Stress Exposure on the Short-Term Memory Capacity in Cadets
PDF Russian
PDF

Keywords

short-term memory
memory capacity
Digit Span Test
goal
cognitive assessment
stressor intensity
self-regulation
emotional stress (arousal)
physiological parameters
stress

Abstract

Introduction. The effects of moderate-intensity stressors on short-term memory are studied, which is relevant to the professional training of first responders (firefighters) when preparing them for emergencies. This study investigates changes in the capacity of short-term memory under stress. Significant differences were found among memory parameters of fire cadets and students of civilian specialties. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first experimental study of the stress impact on the short-term memory capacity that takes into account professional education and training of the respondents. Methods. The study comprised 90 respondents, including 50 fire cadets of the Academy of State Fire Service and 40 students of Moscow universities. The subjects were divided into an experimental group and three control groups. Stressful stimuli (disturbing photo- and audio materials) were presented to respondents from two groups; respondents in two other groups were presented with neutral stimuli. The short-term memory capacity before and after the stressful stimulation was assessed with the Digit Span Test. The physiological signals of the subjects were measured during the experiment. Results. In fire cadets, stress resulted in a significant increase in memory capacity, while memory capacity showed a slight decrease in students. Physiological response to stressful conditions was different between students and fire cadets. Compared to cadets, the baseline muscle tension measures were higher in students. Compared to students, cardiovascular system parameters (systolic wave amplitude, pulse transit time) changed faster in cadets. However, these parameters changed not as substantially as those in students. Discussion. In fire cadets, memory capacity and its change due to stressful effects were associated with levels of non-verbal intelligence. It might also be mediated by emergency professionals' competencies. The system of selection, psychological training, and counseling of fire cadets in the Academy of State Fire Service is considered to be an important factor in the stability of cognitive functions under stress.

https://doi.org/10.21702/rpj.2020.1.3
PDF Russian
PDF

References

Bargh, J. A., & Cohen, J. L. (1978). Mediating factors in the arousal-performance relationship. Motivation and Emotion, 2, 243–257. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00992589

Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4), 553–573. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.553

Bityutskaya, E. V., Eliseeva, I. N., & Shoigu, Yu. S. (2015). To the question of psychological mechanisms of rescuers training for professional activities. In V. T. Kudryavtsev (Ed.), Education and Development: Modern Theory and Practice: Materials of the XVI International Readings in Memory of L. S. Vygotsky. Moscow, November 20, 2015: At 2 p.m. Part 1 (pp. 240–244). Moscow: Lev. (in Russ.).

Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 1–51. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60235-X

Bodrov, V. A., & Oboznov, A. A. (2000). The system of mental regulation of stress tolerance of a human operator. Psikhologicheskii zhurnal (Psychological Journal), 21(4), 32–40. (in Russ.).

Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15(7), 482–487. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00705.x

Buchanan, T. W., & Lovallo, W. R. (2001). Enhanced memory for emotional material following stress-level cortisol treatment in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26(3), 307–317. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4530(00)00058-5

Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2006). Impaired memory retrieval correlates with individual differences in cortisol response but not autonomic response. Learning & Memory, 13, 382–387. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.206306

Cahill, L., Gorski, L., & Le, K. (2003). Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: Interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learning & Memory, 10, 270–274. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.62403

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Crum, A. J., Salovey, P., & Achor, S. (2013). Rethinking stress: The role of mindsets in determining the stress response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 716–733. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031201

Dikaya, L. G. (1999). System-activity concept of self-regulation of the psychophysiological state of a person. In L. G. Dikaya (Ed.), Theory and methods of psychological analysis (pp. 80–106). Moscow: Institute of Psychology RAS. (in Russ.).

Dikaya, L. G., & Shchedrov, V. I. (1999). A method for determining the individual style of self-regulation of a person’s mental state. In L. G. Dikaya (Ed.), Theory and methods of psychological analysis (pp. 106–132). Moscow: Institute of Psychology RAS. (in Russ.).

Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3), 183–201. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0047707

Gurenkova, T. N., Eliseeva, I. N., Kuznetsova, T. Yu., Makarova, O. L., Matafonova, T. Yu., Pavlova, M. V., & Shoigu, Yu. S. (2007). In Yu. S. Shoigu (Ed.), Psychology of extreme situations for rescuers and firefighters. Moscow: Smysl. (in Russ.).

Hancock, P. A., & Weaver, J. L. (2005). On time distortion under stress. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 6(2), 193–211. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14639220512331325747

Hase, A., O'Brien, J., Moore, L. J., & Freeman, P. (2019). The relationship between challenge and threat states and performance: A systematic review. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 8(2), 123–144. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spy0000132

Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 208–212. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.015

Jones, G., & Macken, B. (2015). Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: Why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition, 144, 1–13. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.009

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46(8), 819–834. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.46.8.819

Leonova, A. B., & Kuznetsova, A. S. (2015). Functional conditions and human performance in professional activities. In E. A. Klimov, O. G. Noskova, G. N. Solntseva (Eds.), Labor Psychology, Engineering Psychology and Ergonomics (pp. 319–346). Moscow: Yurayt. (in Russ.).

Levine, L. J., & Burgess, S. L. (1997). Beyond general arousal: Effects of specific emotions on memory. Social Cognition, 15(3), 157–181. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.1997.15.3.157

Levine, L. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2004). Emotion and memory research: A grumpy overview. Social Cognition, 22(5), 530–554. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.22.5.530.50767

Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(1), 33–52. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00028.x

McGaugh, J. L. (2006). Make mild moments memorable: add a little arousal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 345–347. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.001

Shapkin, S. A. (1999). A technique for studying strategies for adapting a person to the stressful conditions of professional activity. A method for determining the individual style of self-regulation of a person’s mental state. In L. G. Dikaya (Ed.), Theory and methods of psychological analysis (pp. 132–165). Moscow: Institute of Psychology RAS. (in Russ.).

Sharot, T., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: Disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 294–306. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/cabn.4.3.294

Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R. M., & Leitten, C. L. (1993). Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 248–260. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.248

Velichkovsky, B. B. (2016). The influence of working memory on the effectiveness of self-regulation of behavior and coping. In T. L. Kryukova, M. V. Saporovskaya, S. A. Khazova (Eds.), Psychology of stress and coping behavior: resources, health, development: Materials of the IV Intern. scientific conf. Kostroma, September 22–24. 2016: In 2 volumes (pp. 25–27). Kostroma: KSU named after N. A. Nekrasov. (in Russ.).

Vine, S. J., Freeman, P., Moore, L. J., Chandra-Ramanan, R., & Wilson, M. R. (2013). Evaluating stress as a challenge is associated with superior attentional control and motor skill performance: Testing the predictions of the biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19(3), 185–194. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034106

Vine, S. J., Uiga, L., Lavric, A., Moore, L. J., Tsaneva-Atanasova, K., & Wilson, M. R. (2015). Individual reactions to stress predict performance during a critical aviation incident. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 28(4), 467–477. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2014.986722

Zotov, M. V. (2011). The mechanisms of regulation of cognitive activity in conditions of emotional stress. St. Petersburg: Rech’. (in Russ.).